References
Abbot, E. E. (1909). On the analysis of the memory consciousness in orthography. Psychol. Monogr., 11(1), 127–158.
Alba, J. W., & Hasher, L. (1983). Is memory schematic? Psychol. Bull., 93(2), 203–231.
Allan, K., & Gabbert, F. (2008). I still think it was a banana: Memorable “lies” and forgettable “truths.” Acta Psychologica, 127(2), 299–308.
Allen, S. W., & Brooks, L. R. (1991). Specializing the operation of an explicit rule. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 120(1), 3–19.
Andrews, I. (1982). Bilinguals out of focus: A critical discussion. International Review of Applied Linguistics in Language Teaching, 20(4), 297–305.
Apperly, I. A., & Butterfill, S. A. (2009). Do humans have two systems to track beliefs and belief-like states? Psychological Review, 116 4, 953–970.
Association, A. P. (2000). Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders (4eme ed.) washington.
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1968). Human memory: A proposed system and its control processes. In Psychology of learning and motivation (Vol. 2, pp. 89–195). Elsevier.
Atkinson, R. C., & Shiffrin, R. M. (1971). The control of short-term memory. Scientific American, 225(2), 82–91.
Baddeley, A. D. (1990). The development of the concept of working memory: Implications and contributions of neuropsychology. In G. Vallar & T. Shallice (Eds.), Neuropsychological impairments of Short-Term memory (pp. 54–73). Cambridge University Press.
Baddeley, A. D. (2000). The episodic buffer: A new component of working memory? Trends Cogn. Sci., 4(11), 417–423.
Baddeley, A. D., & Hitch, G. (1974). Working memory (G. H. Bower, Ed.; Vol. 8, pp. 47–89). Academic Press.
Baddeley, A. D., Thomson, N., & Buchanan, M. (1975). Word length and the structure of short-term memory. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 14(6), 575–589.
Baird, J. A., & Baldwin, D. A. (2001). Making sense of human behavior: Action parsing and intentional inference. In B. F. Malle, L. J. Moses, & D. A. Baldwin (Eds.), Intentions and intentionality: Foundations of social cognition (pp. 193–206). MIT Press.
Baldwin, D. A. (1993). Early referential understanding: Infants’ ability to recognize referential acts for what they are. Dev. Psychol., 29(5), 832–843.
Baldwin, D. A., & Tomasello, M. (1998). Word learning: A window on early pragmatic understanding. In E. V. Clark (Ed.), The proceedings of the twenty-ninth annual child language research forum (pp. 3–23). Center for the Study of Language; Information.
Bargh, J. A. (2014). Our unconscious mind. Sci. Am., 310(1), 30–37.
Bartlett, J. C., & Memon, A. (2007). Eyewitness memory in young and older adults. Lawrence Erlbaum Mahwah, NJ.
Beilock, S. L., & Carr, T. H. (2001). On the fragility of skilled performance: What governs choking under pressure? J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 130(4), 701–725.
Benjamin, A. S., & Tullis, J. (2010). What makes distributed practice effective? Cognitive Psychology, 61, 228–247. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cogpsych.2010.05.004.What
Benjamin, L. T. (2007). A brief history of modern psychology. Blackwell Publishing.
Berkowitz, S. R., Laney, C., Morris, E. K., Garry, M., & Loftus, E. F. (2008). Pluto behaving badly: False beliefs and their consequences. Am. J. Psychol., 121(4), 643–660.
Bernstein, D. M., Laney, C., Morris, E. K., & Loftus, E. F. (2005). False memories about food can lead to food avoidance. Soc. Cogn., 23(1), 11–34.
Bernstein, D. M., & Loftus, E. F. (2009a). The consequences of false memories for food preferences and choices. Perspect. Psychol. Sci., 4(2), 135–139.
Bernstein, D. M., & Loftus, E. F. (2009b). How to tell if a particular memory is true or false. Perspect. Psychol. Sci., 4(4), 370–374.
Bialystok, E. (2009). Bilingualism: The good, the bad, and the indifferent. Biling. (Camb. Engl.), 12(1), 3–11.
Bogdan, R. (2000). Minding minds: Evolving a reflexive mind by interpreting others. MIT Press.
Bornstein, B. H., Deffenbacher, K. A., Penrod, S. D., & McGorty, E. K. (2012). Effects of exposure time and cognitive operations on facial identification accuracy: A meta-analysis of two variables associated with initial memory strength. Psychology, Crime & Law, 18(5), 473–490.
Bower, G. H. (1981). Mood and memory. American Psychologist, 36(2), 129.
Boysson-Bardies, B. de, Sagart, L., & Durand, C. (1984). Discernible differences in the babbling of infants according to target language. Journal of Child Language, 11(1), 1–15.
Brady, T. F., Störmer, V. S., & Alvarez, G. A. (2016). Working memory is not fixed-capacity: More active storage capacity for real-world objects than for simple stimuli. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 113(27), 7459–7464.
Braun, K. A., Ellis, R., & Loftus, E. F. (2002). Make my memory: How advertising can change our memories of the past. Psychol. Mark., 19(1), 1–23.
Bressan, P., & Dal Martello, M. F. (2002). Talis pater, talis filius: Perceived resemblance and the belief in genetic relatedness. Psychological Science, 13, 213–218.
Brewer, W. F., & Treyens, J. C. (1981). Role of schemata in memory for places. Cognitive Psychology, 13(2), 207–230.
Bridgeman, B., & Morgan, R. (1996). Success in college for students with discrepancies between performance on multiple-choice and essay tests. J. Educ. Psychol., 88(2), 333–340.
Brigham, J. C., Bennett, L. B., Meissner, C. A., & Mitchell, T. L. (2007). The influence of race on eyewitness memory.
Brigham, J. C., Ready, D. J., & Spier, S. A. (1990). Standards for evaluating the fairness of photograph lineups. Basic and Applied Social Psychology, 11(2), 149–163.
Broadbent, D. E., & Dal Martello, M. F. (1958). Perception and communication. Psychological Science.
Brooks, L. R. (1968). Spatial and verbal components of the act of recall. Can. J. Psychol., 22(5), 349–368.
Brown, A. S. (1991). A review of tip of the tongue experience. Psychological Bulletin, 109, 79–91.
Brown, J. R. (1967). Biological foundations of language. Neurology, 17(12), 1219–1219.
Brown, R., & Kulik, J. (1977). Flashbulb memories. Cognition, 5(1), 73–99.
Burton, A. M., Wilson, S., Cowan, M., & Bruce, V. (1999). Face recognition in poor-quality video: Evidence from security surveillance. Psychological Science, 10(3), 243–248.
Byrne, R. M. J., & Johnson-Laird, P. N. (2009). “If” and the problems of conditional reasoning. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 13(7), 282–287.
Calvo, P., & Gomila, T. (2008). Handbook of cognitive science: An embodied approach. Elsevier.
Caputo, D., & Dunning, D. (2007). Distinguishing accurate identifications from erroneous ones: Post-dictive indicators of eyewitness accuracy. In The handbook of eyewitness psychology: Volume II (pp. 441–464). Psychology Press.
Caselli, N., Pyers, J., & Lieberman, A. M. (2021). Deaf children of hearing parents have age-level vocabulary growth when exposed to american sign language by 6 months of age. The Journal of Pediatrics, 232, 229–236.
Ceci, S. J., & Bruck, M. (1995). Jeopardy in the courtroom: A scientific analysis of children’s testimony. American Psychological Association.
Cepeda, N. J., Vul, E., Rohrer, D., Wixted, J. T., & Pashler, H. (2008). Spacing effects in learning: A temporal ridgeline of optimal retention. Psychological Science, 19, 1095–1102. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-9280.2008.02209.x
Chartrand, T. L., & Bargh, J. A. (1999). The chameleon effect: The perception–behavior link and social interaction. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 76, 893–910.
Chase, W. G., & Simon, H. A. (1973). Perception in chess. Cogn. Psychol., 4(1), 55–81.
Cheesman, J., & Merikle, P. M. (1984). Priming with and without awareness. Percept. Psychophys., 36(4), 387–395.
Cheesman, J., & Merikle, P. M. (1986). Distinguishing conscious from unconscious perceptual processes. Can. J. Psychol., 40(4), 343–367.
Cheng, P. W., & Holyoak, K. J. (1985). Pragmatic reasoning schemas. Cognitive Psychology, 17(4), 391–416.
Chernev, A., Böckenholt, U., & Goodman, J. (2015). Choice overload: A conceptual review and meta-analysis. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 25(2), 333–358.
Cherry, E. C. (1953). Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. Journal of the Acoustical Society of America, 25, 975–979.
Chi, M. T. H., Feltovich, P. J., & Glaser, R. (1981). Categorization and representation of physics problems by experts and novices. Cogn. Sci., 5(2), 121–152.
Chiao, J. (2009). Culture–gene coevolution of individualism – collectivism and the serotonin transporter gene. Proceedings of the Royal Society B, 277, 529–537. https://doi.org/10.1098/rspb.2009.1650
Choi, J. J., Laibson, D., Madrian, B. C., & Metrick, A. (2002). Defined contribution pensions: Plan rules, participant choices, and the path of least resistance. Tax Policy and the Economy, 16, 67–113.
Conrad, R. (1964). Acoustic confusions in immediate memory. British Journal of Psychology, 55(1), 75–84.
Cosmides, L. (1989). The logic of social exchange: Has natural selection shaped how humans reason? Studies with the wason selection task. Cognition, 31(3), 187–276.
Craik, F. I. M., & Lockhart, R. S. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 11(6), 671–684.
Craik, F. I. M., & Tulving, E. (1975). Depth of processing and the retention of words in episodic memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 104(3), 268–294.
Cutler, B. L., & Penrod, S. D. (1995). Mistaken identification: The eyewitness, psychology and the law. Cambridge University Press.
Czaykowska-Higgins, E., Burton, S., McIvor, O., & Marinakis, A. (2017). Supporting indigenous language revitalisation through collaborative postsecondary proficiency-building curriculum.
Darley, J. M., & Gross, P. H. (1983a). A hypothesis-confirming bias in labeling effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44, 20–33.
Darley, J. M., & Gross, P. H. (1983b). A hypothesis-confirming bias in labeling effects. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 44(1), 20.
De Waal, F. B. (1989). Peacemaking among primates. Harvard University Press.
Deffenbacher, K. A., Bornstein, B. H., Penrod, S. D., & McGorty, E. K. (2004). A meta-analytic review of the effects of high stress on eyewitness memory. Law and Human Behavior, 28(6), 687.
Deutsch, J. A., & Deutsch, D. (1963). Attention: Some theoretical considerations. Psychol. Rev., 70(1), 80–90.
Dick, A. S., Garcia, N. L., Pruden, S. M., Thompson, W. K., Hawes, S. W., Sutherland, M. T., Riedel, M. C., Laird, A. R., & Gonzalez, R. (2019). No evidence for a bilingual executive function advantage in the ABCD study. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(7), 692–701.
Dinner, I., Johnson, E. J., Goldstein, D. G., & Liu, K. (2011). Partitioning default effects: Why people choose not to choose. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 17(4), 332.
Ditta, A. S., & Storm, B. C. (2018). A consideration of the seven sins of memory in the context of creative cognition. Creativity Research Journal, 30(4), 402–417.
Dobrich, W., & Scarborough, H. S. (1992). Phonological characteristics of words young children try to say. J. Child Lang., 19(3), 597–616.
Dominowski, R. L., & Dallob, P. (1995). Insight and problem solving.
Duncker, K. (1945). On problem-solving. Psychological Monographs, 58(5), i.
Dunn, E. W., Aknin, L. B., & Norton, M. I. (2008). Spending money on others promotes happiness. Science, 319(5870), 1687–1688. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1150952
Enns, J. T., & Lleras, A. (2008). What’s next? New evidence for prediction in human vision. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(9), 327–333.
Epley, N., Morewedge, C. K., & Keysar, B. (2004). Perspective taking in children and adults: Equivalent egocentrism but differential correction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 40, 760–768.
Fancher, R. E. (1987). The intelligence men: Makers of the IQ controversy. W.W. Norton & Company.
Flanagan, M. B., May, J. G., & Dobie, T. G. (2004). The role of vection, eye movements, and postural instability in the etiology of motion sickness. Journal of Vestibular Research: Equilibrium and Orientation, 14(4), 335–346.
Frenda, S. J., Nichols, R. M., & Loftus, E. F. (2011). Current issues and advances in misinformation research. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 20(1), 20–23.
Fuchs, A. H. (2000). Contributions of american mental philosophers to psychology in the united states. History of Psychology, 3, 3–19.
Gabbert, F., Memon, A., & Allan, K. (2003). Memory conformity: Can eyewitnesses influence each other’s memories for an event? Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 17(5), 533–543.
Gabbert, F., Memon, A., Allan, K., & Wright, D. B. (2004). Say it to my face: Examining the effects of socially encountered misinformation. Legal and Criminological Psychology, 9(2), 215–227.
Gais, S., Lucas, B., & Born, J. (2006). Sleep after learning aids memory recall. Learning & Memory, 13(3), 259–262.
Galanter, E. (1962). Contemporary psychophysics. In r. Brown, e. Galanter, e. H. Hess, & g. Mandler (eds.), new directions in psychology. Holt, Rinehart; Winston.
Garrod, S., & Sanford, A. (1977). Interpreting anaphoric relations: The integration of semantic information while reading. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 16(1), 77–90.
Garry, M., French, L., Kinzett, T., & Mori, K. (2008). Eyewitness memory following discussion: Using the MORI technique with a western sample. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 22(4), 431–439.
George, T., & Wiley, J. (2020). Need something different? Here’s what’s been done: Effects of examples and task instructions on creative idea generation. Memory & Cognition, 48, 226–243.
Gergely, G., & Csibra, G. (2003). Teleological reasoning in infancy: The naïve theory of rational action. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7, 287–292.
Gergely, G., Nádasdy, Z., Csibra, G., & Bíró, S. (1995). Taking the intentional stance at 12 months of age. Cognition, 56(2), 165–193.
Gibson, E. J., Pick, A. D., et al. (2000). An ecological approach to perceptual learning and development. Oxford University Press, USA.
Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1980). Analogical problem solving. Cognitive Psychology, 12(3), 306–355.
Gick, M. L., & Holyoak, K. J. (1983). Schema induction and analogical transfer. Cognitive Psychology, 15(1), 1–38.
Gigerenzer, G., & Hug, K. (1992). Domain-specific reasoning: Social contracts, cheating, and perspective change. Cognition, 43(2), 127–171.
Gilovich, T., & Savitsky, K. (1999). The spotlight effect and the illusion of transparency: Egocentric assessments of how we are seen by others. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 8, 165–168.
Godden, D. R., & Baddeley, A. D. (1975). Context-dependent memory in two natural environments: On land and underwater. Br. J. Psychol., 66(3), 325–331.
Goldstein, E. (2005). Cognitive psychology - connecting, mind research, and everyday experience. Thomson Wadsworth.
Goodale, M., & Milner, D. (2006). One brain-two visual systems. Psychologist, 19(11), 660–663.
Green, L. (2002). A descriptive study of african american english: Research in linguistics and education. International Journal of Qualitative Studies in Education, 15(6), 673–690.
Greenfield, P. M., & Savage-Rumbaugh, E. S. (1991). Imitation, grammatical development, and the invention of protogrammar by an ape. In N. A. Krasnegor, D. M. Rumbaugh, R. L. Schiefelbusch, & &. M. Studdert-Kennedy (Eds.), Biological and behavioral determinants of language development (pp. 235–258). Lawrence Erlbaum Associates.
Greenwald, A. G. (1992). New look 3: Unconscious cognition reclaimed. Am. Psychol., 47(6), 766–779.
Griggs, R. A., & Cox, J. R. (1982). The elusive thematic-materials effect in wason’s selection task. British Journal of Psychology, 73(3), 407–420.
Griggs, R. A., & Cox, J. R. (1983). The effects of problem content and negation on wason’s selection task. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 35(3), 519–533.
Guglielmo, S., Monroe, A. E., & Malle, B. F. (2009). At the heart of morality lies folk psychology. Inquiry: An Interdisciplinary Journal of Philosophy, 52, 449–466.
Gupta, M. W., Pan, S. C., & Rickard, T. C. (2022). Prior episodic learning and the efficacy of retrieval practice. Memory & Cognition, 50(4), 722–735.
Haist, F., Shimamura, A. P., & Squire, L. R. (1992). On the relationship between recall and recognition memory. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 18(4), 691–702.
Hakuta, K., Bialystok, E., & Wiley, E. (2003). Critical evidence: A test of the critical-period hypothesis for second-language acquisition. Psychol. Sci., 14(1), 31–38.
Hall, M. L., Hall, W. C., & Caselli, N. K. (2019). Deaf children need language, not (just) speech. First Language, 39(4), 367–395.
Hall, W. C. (2017). What you don’t know can hurt you: The risk of language deprivation by impairing sign language development in deaf children. Maternal and Child Health Journal, 21(5), 961–965.
Hampton, J. A. (1979). Polymorphous concepts in semantic memory. J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behav., 18(4), 441–461.
Harris, C. R., Coburn, N., Rohrer, D., & Pashler, H. (2013). Two failures to replicate high-performance-goal priming effects. PloS One, 8(8), e72467.
Hartung, C. M., & Widiger, T. A. (1998). Gender differences in the diagnosis of mental disorders: Conclusions and controversies of the DSM–IV. Psychological Bulletin, 123(3), 260.
Heaps, C., & Nash, M. (1999). Individual differences in imagination inflation. Psychon. Bull. Rev., 6(2), 313–318.
Henley, T. B., & Thorne, B. M. (2005). The lost millennium: Psychology during the middle ages. The Psychological Record, 55(1), 103–113.
Howard, I. P. (2002). Seeing in depth, vol. 1: Basic mechanisms. University of Toronto Press.
Hu, X., Cheng, L. Y., Chiu, M. H., & Paller, K. A. (2020). Promoting memory consolidation during sleep: A meta-analysis of targeted memory reactivation. Psychol. Bull., 146(3), 218–244.
Humphries, T., Kushalnagar, P., Mathur, G., Napoli, D. J., Padden, C., Rathmann, C., & Smith, S. (2016). Avoiding linguistic neglect of deaf children. Social Service Review, 90(4), 589–619.
Hyman, I. E., Husband, T. H., & Billings, F. J. (1995). False memories of childhood experiences. Appl. Cogn. Psychol., 9(3), 181–197.
Iyengar, S. S., & Lepper, M. R. (2000). When choice is demotivating: Can one desire too much of a good thing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 79(6), 995.
Jachimowicz, J. M., Duncan, S., Weber, E. U., & Johnson, E. J. (2019). When and why defaults influence decisions: A meta-analysis of default effects. Behavioural Public Policy, 3(2), 159–186.
Jackson, A., Koek, W., & Colpaert, F. C. (1992). NMDA antagonists make learning and recall state-dependent. Behav. Pharmacol., 3(4), 415–421.
Jacoby, L. L., & Rhodes, M. G. (2006). False remembering in the aged. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 15(2), 49–53.
James, W. (1890). Principles of psychology.
Jara-Ettinger, J., Gweon, H., Schulz, L. E., & Tenenbaum, J. B. (2016). The naïve utility calculus: Computational principles underlying commonsense psychology. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 20(8), 589–604.
Johnson, E. J., & Goldstein, D. (2003). Do defaults save lives? In Science (No. 5649; Vol. 302, pp. 1338–1339). American Association for the Advancement of Science.
Johnson, J. S., & Newport, E. L. (1989). Critical period effects in second language learning: The influence of maturational state on the acquisition of english as a second language. Cogn. Psychol., 21(1), 60–99.
Johnson, S. C. (2000). The recognition of mentalistic agents in infancy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 22–28.
Johnson-Laird, P. (1985). Mental models: Towards a cognitive science of language, inference, and consciousness / p.n. Johnson-laird. Language, 61.
Johnson-Laird, P., & Byrne, R. M. J. (1991). Deduction. Psychology Press.
Johnson-Laird, P., & Byrne, R. M. J. (2002). Conditionals: A theory of meaning, pragmatics, and inference. Psychological Review, 109(4), 646–678.
Johnston, W. A., & Heinz, S. P. (1978). Flexibility and capacity demands of attention. J. Exp. Psychol. Gen., 107(4), 420–435.
Jones, M. V., Paull, G. C., & Erskine, J. (2002). The impact of a team’s aggressive reputation on the decisions of association football referees. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20, 991–1000.
Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect theory: An analysis of decisions under risk. Econometrica, 47, 278.
Karpicke, J. D., Lehman, M., & Aue, W. R. (2014). Retrieval-based learning. An episodic context account. In Psychology of Learning and Motivation - Advances in Research and Theory (Vol. 61, pp. 237–284). https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-12-800283-4.00007-1
Keysar, B. (1994). The illusory transparency of intention: Linguistic perspective taking in text. Cognitive Psychology, 26, 165–208.
Kounios, J., & Beeman, M. (2009). The aha! Moment: The cognitive neuroscience of insight. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 18(4), 210–216.
Kraft, C. (1978). A psychophysical approach to air safety: Simulator studies of visual illusions in night approaches. In h. L. Pick, h. W. Leibowitz, j. E. Singer, a. Steinschneider, & h. W. Steenson (eds.), psychology: From research to practice (2nd ed.). Plenum Press.
Kromann, C. B., Jensen, M. L., & Ringsted, C. (2009). The effect of testing on skills learning. Medical Education, 43, 21–27. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2008.03245.x
Krueger, J. I. (2007). From social projection to social behaviour. European Review of Social Psychology, 18, 1–35.
Kudrnova, V., Spelke, E. S., & Thomas, A. J. (2024). Infants infer social relationships between individuals who engage in imitative social interactions. Open Mind: Discoveries in Cognitive Science, 8, 202–216.
Laney, C., & Loftus, E. F. (2008). Emotional content of true and false memories. Memory, 16(5), 500–516.
Lantz, M. E. (2010). The use of ’clickers’ in the classroom: Teaching innovation or merely an amusing novelty? Computers in Human Behavior, 26, 556–561. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.chb.2010.02.014
Larsen, D. P., Butler, A. C., & Roediger, H. L. (2009). Repeated testing improves long-term retention relative to repeated study: A randomised controlled trial. Medical Education, 43, 1174–1181. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1365-2923.2009.03518.x
Laurence, S., & Margolis, E. (1999). Concepts and cognitive science. In E. Margolis & S. Laurence (Eds.), Concepts: Core readings (pp. 3–81). MIT Press.
Lee, J., & Strayer, D. (2004). Preface to the special section on driver distraction. Human Factors, 46(4), 583.
Levin, I. P., Schneider, S. L., & Gaeth, G. J. (1998). All frames are not created equal: A typology and critical analysis of framing effects. Organizational Behavior and Human Decision Processes, 76(2), 149–188.
Lichtenstein, S., Slovic, P., Fischhoff, B., Layman, M., & Combs, B. (1978). Judged frequency of lethal events. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4(6), 551.
Lillo-Martin, D., & Henner, J. (2021). Acquisition of sign languages. Annual Review of Linguistics, 7, 395.
Lindsay, D. S., Hagen, L., Read, J. D., Wade, K. A., & Garry, M. (2004). True photographs and false memories. Psychol. Sci., 15(3), 149–154.
Liu, S., Pepe, B., Kumar, M. G., Ullman, T. D., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Spelke, E. S. (2022). Dangerous ground: One-year-old infants are sensitive to peril in other agents’ action plans. Open Mind, 6, 211–231.
Liu, S., Ullman, T. D., Tenenbaum, J. B., & Spelke, E. S. (2017). Ten-month-old infants infer the value of goals from the costs of actions. Science, 358(6366), 1038–1041.
Livingstone, M. S. (2000). Is it warm? Is it real? Or just low spatial frequency? Science, 290(5495), 1299–1299.
Loftus, E. F. (1975). Leading questions and the eyewitness report. Cognitive Psychology, 7(4), 560–572.
Loftus, E. F. (2005). Planting misinformation in the human mind: A 30-year investigation of the malleability of memory. Learning & Memory, 12(4), 361–366.
Loftus, E. F., & Ketcham, K. (1996). The myth of repressed memory: False memories and allegations of sexual abuse. Macmillan.
Loftus, E. F., Miller, D. G., & Burns, H. J. (1978). Semantic integration of verbal information into a visual memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 4(1), 19.
Loftus, E. F., & Pickrell, J. E. (1995). The formation of false memories. Psychiatr. Ann., 25(12), 720–725.
Lohnas, L. J., & Kahana, M. J. (2014). A retrieved context account of spacing and repetition effects in free recall. J Exp Psychol Learn Mem Cogn, 40, 755–764. https://doi.org/10.1038/jid.2014.371
Lucas, R. E., Clark, A. E., Georgellis, Y., & Diener, E. (2003). Re-examining adaptation and the setpoint model of happiness: Reactions to changes in marital status. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 84, 527–539.
Luchins, A. S. (1942). Mechanization in problem solving: The effect of einstellung. Psychological Monographs, 54(6), i.
Lukaniec, M., & Palakurthy, K. (2022). Additional language learning in the context of indigenous language reclamation. In The routledge handbook of second language acquisition and sociolinguistics (pp. 341–355). Routledge.
MacKenzie, A., Engman, M., & McGurk, O. (2022). Overt and symbolic linguistic violence: Plantation ideology and language reclamation in northern ireland. Teaching in Higher Education, 27(4), 489–501.
Maier, N. R. (1930). Reasoning in humans. I. On direction. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 10(2), 115.
Maier, N. R. (1931). Reasoning in humans. II. The solution of a problem and its appearance in consciousness. Journal of Comparative Psychology, 12(2), 181.
Malle, B. F. (2008). The fundamental tools, and possibly universals, of social cognition. In R. M. Sorrentino & S. Yamaguchi (Eds.), Handbook of motivation and cognition across cultures (pp. 267–296). Elsevier/Academic Press.
Malle, B. F., & Knobe, J. (1997). The folk concept of intentionality. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 33, 101–121.
Mandel, D. R., Jusczyk, P. W., & Pisoni, D. B. (1995). Infants’ recognition of the sound patterns of their own names. Psychol. Sci., 6(5), 314–317.
Maran, M. (2010). My lie: A true story of false memory. John Wiley & Sons.
Mareschal, D., Quinn, P. C., & Lea, S. E. G. (2010). The making of human concepts: A final look. In The making of human concepts (pp. 387–394). Oxford University Press.
Marian, V., & Kaushanskaya, M. (2007). Language context guides memory content. Psychon. Bull. Rev., 14(5), 925–933.
Mauldin, L. (2019). Don’t look at it as a miracle cure: Contested notions of success and failure in family narratives of pediatric cochlear implantation. Social Science & Medicine, 228, 117–125.
Mazzoni, G. A. L., Loftus, E. F., Seitz, A., & Lynn, S. J. (1999). Changing beliefs and memories through dream interpretation. Appl. Cogn. Psychol., 13(2), 125–144.
McCann, J. J. (1992). Rules for color constancy. Ophthalmic and Physiologic Optics, 12(2), 175–177.
McDaniel, M. A., Anderson, J. L., Derbish, M. H., & Morrisette, N. (2007). Testing the testing effect in the classroom. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology, 19, 494–513. https://doi.org/10.1080/09541440701326154
McIntyre, L. (2019). The scientific attitude: Defending science from denial, fraud, and pseudoscience. Mit Press.
McKenzie, C. R., Liersch, M. J., & Finkelstein, S. R. (2006). Recommendations implicit in policy defaults. Psychological Science, 17(5), 414–420.
McNeil, B. J., Pauker, S. G., Sox Jr, H. C., & Tversky, A. (1982). On the elicitation of preferences for alternative therapies. New England Journal of Medicine, 306(21), 1259–1262.
Mechelli, A., Crinion, J. T., Noppeney, U., O’doherty, J., Ashburner, J., Frackowiak, R. S., & Price, C. J. (2004). Structural plasticity in the bilingual brain: Proficiency in a second language and age at acquisition affect grey-matter density. Nature.
Medin, D. L., & Schaffer, M. M. (1978). Context theory of classification learning. Psychol. Rev., 85(3), 207–238.
Meltzoff, A. N. (2007). “Like me”: A foundation for social cognition. Developmental Science, 10, 126–134.
Meltzoff, A. N., & Decety, J. (2003). What imitation tells us about social cognition: A rapprochement between developmental psychology and cognitive neuroscience. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London. Series B: Biological Sciences, 358, 491–500.
Merikle, P. (2004). Subliminal perception. In Encyclopedia of psychology, vol. 7 (pp. 497–499). American Psychological Association.
Mervis, C. B., & Pani, J. R. (1980). Acquisition of basic object categories. Cognitive Psychology, 12(4), 496–522.
Meyer, D. E., & Schvaneveldt, R. W. (1971). Facilitation in recognizing pairs of words: Evidence of a dependence between retrieval operations. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 90(2), 227.
Miller, G. A. (2003). The cognitive revolution: A historical perspective. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 141–144.
Mitchell, R. E., & Karchmer, M. (2004). Chasing the mythical ten percent: Parental hearing status of deaf and hard of hearing students in the united states. Sign Language Studies, 4(2), 138–163.
Miyake, A., Friedman, N. P., Emerson, M. J., Witzki, A. H., Howerter, A., & Wager, T. D. (2000). The unity and diversity of executive functions and their contributions to complex “frontal lobe” tasks: A latent variable analysis. Cogn. Psychol., 41(1), 49–100.
Monsell, S. (2003). Task switching. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 7(3), 134–140.
Moon, C., Cooper, R. P., & Fifer, W. P. (1993). Two-day-olds prefer their native language. Infant Behav. Dev., 16(4), 495–500.
Moray, N. (1959). Attention in dichotic listening: Affective cues and the influence of instructions. Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 11(1), 56–60.
Morris, C. D., Bransford, J. D., & Franks, J. J. (1977). Levels of processing versus transfer appropriate processing. J. Verbal Learning Verbal Behav., 16(5), 519–533.
Mozer, M. C., Pashler, H., Cepeda, N., Lindsey, R., & Vul, E. (2009). Predicting the optimal spacing of study: A multiscale context model of memory. Advances in Neural Information Processing Systems 22 - Proceedings of the 2009 Conference, 1321–1330.
Murphy, G. (2002). The big book of concepts.
Neisser, U. (1979). The control of information pickup in selective looking. In Perception and its development (pp. 201–219). Psychology Press.
Nickerson, R. S. (1998a). Applied psychology: An international review. Applied Experimental Psychology, 47, 155–173.
Nickerson, R. S. (1998b). Confirmation bias: A ubiquitous phenomenon in many guises. Review of General Psychology, 2(2), 175–220.
Nickerson, R. S., & Adams, M. J. (1979). Long-term memory for a common object. Cogn. Psychol., 11(3), 287–307.
Nicoladis, E., & Genesee, F. (1997). Language development in preschool bilingual children. Journal of Speech-Language Pathology and Audiology, 21(4), 258–270.
NIDCD, N. (2021). Cochlear implants. National Institute on Deafness; Other Communication Disorders.
Nitschke, J. B., Dixon, G. E., Sarinopoulos, I., Short, S. J., Cohen, J. D., Smith, E. E., Kosslyn, S. M., Rose, R. M., & Davidson, R. J. (2006). Altering expectancy dampens neural response to aversive taste in primary taste cortex. Nature Neuroscience, 9(3), 435–442.
Nungester, R. J., & Duchastel, P. C. (1982). Testing versus review: Effects on retention. Journal of Educational Psychology, 74, 18–22. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-0663.74.1.18
Oller, D. K., & Pearson, B. Z. (2002). Chapter 1: Assessing the effects of bilingualism: A background. In K. Oller & R. Eilers (Eds.), Language and literacy in bilingual children (pp. 3–21). Multilingual Matters.
Onishi, K. H., Murphy, G. L., & Bock, K. (2008). Prototypicality in sentence production. Cognitive Psychology, 56(2), 103–141.
Oreskes, N. (2019). Why trust science?
Padilla, L., Hosseinpour, H., Fygenson, R., Howell, J., Chunara, R., & Bertini, E. (2022). Impact of COVID-19 forecast visualizations on pandemic risk perceptions. Scientific Reports, 12(1), 2014.
Pan, S. C., Rubin, B. R., & Rickard, T. C. (2015). Does testing with feedback improve adult spelling skills relative to copying and reading? Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 21, 356–369. https://doi.org/10.1037/xap0000062
Pashler, H. (1993). Doing two things at the same time. American Scientist, 81(1), 48–55.
Paterson, H. M., & Kemp, R. I. (2006). Co-witnesses talk: A survey of eyewitness discussion. Psychology, Crime & Law, 12(2), 181–191.
Penfield, W., & Roberts, L. (1959). Speech & brain mechanisms princeton, NJ princeton uni. Press.
Pepe, B., & Powell, L. J. (2023). Infants’ expectations for prosociality in imitators. Open Science Framework.
Perner, J. (1991). Understanding the representational mind. MIT Press.
Petitto, L. A., & Marentette, P. F. (1991). Babbling in the manual mode: Evidence for the ontogeny of language. Science, 251(5000), 1493–1496.
Pickering, M. J., & Ferreira, V. S. (2008). Structural priming: A critical review. Psychological Bulletin, 134(3), 427.
Pines, M. (1981). The civilizing of genie. Psychology Today, 15(9), 28–34.
Plassmann, H., O’doherty, J., Shiv, B., & Rangel, A. (2008). Marketing actions can modulate neural representations of experienced pleasantness. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 105(3), 1050–1054.
Polyn, S. M., Norman, K. A., & Kahana, M. J. (2009). A context maintenance and retrieval model of organization. Psychology Review, 116, 129–156. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0014420.A
Porter, S., Yuille, J. C., & Lehman, D. R. (1999). The nature of real, implanted, and fabricated memories for emotional childhood events: Implications for the recovered memory debate. Law Hum. Behav., 23(5), 517–537.
Powell, L. J. (2022). Adopted utility calculus: Origins of a concept of social affiliation. Perspectives on Psychological Science.
Pratkanis, A. R., Greenwald, A. G., Leippe, M. R., & Baumgardner, M. H. (1988). In search of reliable persuasion effects: III. The sleeper effect is dead: Long live the sleeper effect. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 54(2), 203–218.
Premack, D. (1990). The infant’s theory of self-propelled objects. Cognition, 36, 1–16.
Proctor, R. W., & Van Zandt, T. (2008). Human factors in simple and complex systems. (2nd ed.). CRC Press.
Project, I. (2023). Innocence project. Retrieved from https://www.innocenceproject.org/all-cases/.
Quillian, M. R. (1967). Word concepts: A theory and simulation of some basic semantic capabilities. Behavioral Science, 12(5), 410–430.
Quinn, P. O., & Madhoo, M. (2014). A review of attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder in women and girls: Uncovering this hidden diagnosis. The Primary Care Companion for CNS Disorders, 16(3), 27250.
Ramachandran, V. S., Hubbard, E. M., Robertson, L. C., & Sagiv, N. (2005). The emergence of the human mind: Some clues from synesthesia. In synesthesia: Perspectives from cognitive neuroscience. (2nd ed., pp. 147–190). Oxford University Press.
Rasch, B., Büchel, C., Gais, S., & Born, J. (2007). Odor cues during slow-wave sleep prompt declarative memory consolidation. Science, 315(5817), 1426–1429.
Rassin, E., Merckelbach, H., & Spaan, V. (2001). When dreams become a royal road to confusion: Realistic dreams, dissociation, and fantasy proneness. J. Nerv. Ment. Dis., 189(7), 478–481.
Rickard, T. C. (2020). Extension of the dual-memory model of test-enhanced learning to distributions and individual differences. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 27(4), 783–790.
Rickard, T. C., & Pan, S. C. (2018). A dual memory theory of the testing effect. Psychonomic Bulletin and Review, 25, 847–869. https://doi.org/10.3758/s13423-017-1298-4
Rips, L. J., Shoben, E. J., & Smith, E. E. (1973). Semantic distance and the verification of semantic relations. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 12(1), 1–20.
Rogers, T. B., Kuiper, N. A., & Kirker, W. S. (1977). Self-reference and the encoding of personal information. J. Pers. Soc. Psychol., 35(9), 677–688.
Rosch, E. H. (1973). On the internal structure of perceptual and semantic categories. In Cognitive development and acquisition of language (pp. 111–144). Elsevier.
Rosch, E., & Mervis, C. B. (1975). Family resemblance: Studies in the internal structure of categories. Cognitive Psychology, 7, 573–605.
Rosch, E., Simpson, C., & Miller, R. S. (1976). Structural bases of typicality effects. J. Exp. Psychol. Hum. Percept. Perform., 2(4), 491–502.
Runco, M. A., & Jaeger, G. J. (2012). The standard definition of creativity. Creativity Research Journal, 24(1), 92–96.
Runeson, S. (1988). The distorted room illusion, equivalent configurations, and the specificity of static optic arrays. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 14(2), 295–304.
Saffran, J. R., Aslin, R. N., & Newport, E. L. (1996). Statistical learning by 8-month-old infants. Science, 274(5294), 1926–1928.
Sapir, E., & Whorf, B. (1956). Language, thought, and reality. Selected Writings.
Savage-Rumbaugh, S., & Lewin, R. (1994). Kanzi: The ape at the brink of the human mind. John Wiley & Sons.
Schacter, D. L. (1999). The seven sins of memory: Insights from psychology and cognitive neuroscience. American Psychologist, 54(3), 182.
Schill, H. M., Wolfe, J. M., & Brady, T. F. (2021). Relationships between expertise and distinctiveness: Abnormal medical images lead to enhanced memory performance only in experts. Memory & Cognition, 49(6), 1067–1081.
Schurgin, M. W., Wixted, J. T., & Brady, T. F. (2020). Psychophysical scaling reveals a unified theory of visual memory strength. Nature Human Behaviour, 4(11), 1156–1172.
Seamon, J. G., Philbin, M. M., & Harrison, L. G. (2006). Do you remember proposing marriage to the pepsi machine? False recollections from a campus walk. Psychon. Bull. Rev., 13(5), 752–756.
Sekuler, R., & Blake, R. (2006). Perception. McGraw-Hill Companies,Incorporated. https://books.google.com/books?id=jKVFAAAAYAAJ
Semenuks, A., Phillips, W., Dalca, I., Kim, C., & Boroditsky, L. (2017). Effects of grammatical gender on object description. CogSci.
Seyfarth, R. M., & Cheney, D. L. (1997). Behavioral mechanisms underlying vocal communication in nonhuman primates. Anim. Learn. Behav., 25(3), 249–267.
Silveira, J. M. (1971). Incubation: The effect of interruption timing and length on problem solution and quality of problem processing. Unpublished doctoral dissertation. University of Oregon.
Silverstein, L. D., Krantz, J. H., Gomer, F. E., Yeh, Y., & Monty, R. W. (1990). The effects of spatial sampling and luminance quantization on the image quality of color matrix displays. Journal of the Optical Society of America, Part A(7), 1955–1968.
Silverstein, L. D., & Merrifield, R. M. (1985). The development and evaluation of color systems for airborne applications: Phase i fundamental visual, perceptual, and display systems considerations (tech. Report DOT/FAA/PM085019) (5th ed., pp. 1955–1968). Federal Aviation Administration.
Simons, D. J., & Chabris, C. F. (1999). Gorillas in our midst: Sustained inattentional blindness for dynamic events. Perception, 28(9), 1059–1074.
Simonton, D. K. (2018). Defining creativity: Don’t we also need to define what is not creative? The Journal of Creative Behavior, 52(1), 80–90.
Skaugset, L. M., Farrell, S., Carney, M., Wolff, M., Santen, S. A., Perry, M., & Cico, S. J. (2016). Can you multitask? Evidence and limitations of task switching and multitasking in emergency medicine. Annals of Emergency Medicine, 68(2), 189–195.
Smith, J. D., & Minda, J. P. (1998). Prototypes in the mist: The early epochs of category learning. J. Exp. Psychol. Learn. Mem. Cogn., 24(6), 1411–1436.
Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Finke, R. A. (1995). The creative cognition approach. MIT press.
Smith, S. M., Ward, T. B., & Schumacher, J. S. (1993). Constraining effects of examples in a creative generation task. Memory & Cognition, 21, 837–845.
Spelke, E., Hirst, W., & Neisser, U. (1976). Skills of divided attention. Cognition, 4(3), 215–230.
Stangor, C., & McMillan, D. (1992). Memory for expectancy-congruent and expectancy-incongruent information: A review of the social and social developmental literatures. Psychological Bulletin, 111(1), 42.
State v. guilbert (Vol. 306, p. 705). (2012). (Vol. 306). Connecticut Supreme Court.
Stern, M., & Karraker, K. H. (1989). Sex stereotyping of infants: A review of gender labeling studies. Sex Roles, 20(9–10), 501–522.
Still, G. F. (1902). The goulstonian lectures. Some Abnormal Psychical Conditions in Children, 1008–1012.
Stoffregen, T. A., & Bardy, B. G. (2001). On specification and the senses. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(2), 195–213.
Strayer, D. L., & Drews, F. A. (2007). Cell-phone induced inattention blindness. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 16, 128–131.
Strayer, D. L., & Johnston, W. A. (2001). Driven to distraction: Dual-task studies of simulated driving and conversing on a cellular telephone. Psychol. Sci., 12(6), 462–466.
Strayer, D. L., Watson, J. M., & Drews, F. A. (2011). Cognitive distraction while multitasking in the automobile. In Advances in research and theory (pp. 29–58). Elsevier.
Stroop, J. R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643.
Takarangi, M. K., Parker, S., & Garry, M. (2006). Modernising the misinformation effect: The development of a new stimulus set. Applied Cognitive Psychology: The Official Journal of the Society for Applied Research in Memory and Cognition, 20(5), 583–590.
Talarico, J. M., & Rubin, D. C. (2003). Confidence, not consistency, characterizes flashbulb memories. Psychological Science, 14(5), 455–461.
Technical Working Group for Eyewitness Evidence. (1999). Eyewitness evidence: A guide for law enforcement. U.S. Department of Justice, Office of Justice Programs.
Thomas, A. J., Woo, B., Nettle, D., Spelke, E., & Saxe, R. (2022). Early concepts of intimacy: Young humans use saliva sharing to infer close relationships. Science, 375(6578), 311–315.
Thomas, A. K., McKinney de Royston, M., & Powell, S. (2023). Color-evasive cognition: The unavoidable impact of scientific racism in the founding of a field. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 32(2), 137–144.
Tiego, J., Testa, R., Bellgrove, M. A., Pantelis, C., & Whittle, S. (2018). A hierarchical model of inhibitory control. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1339.
Tomasello, M. (2003). The key is social cognition. In D. Gentner & S. Goldin-Meadow (Eds.), Language in mind: Advances in the study of language and thought (pp. 47–57). MIT Press.
Treisman, A. M. (1960). Contextual cues in selective listening. Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 12(4), 242–248.
Trope, Y., & Thompson, E. P. (1997). Looking for truth in all the wrong places? Asymmetric search of individuating information about stereotyped group members. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 73(2), 229.
Tulving, E., & Thomson, D. M. (1973). Encoding specificity and retrieval processes in episodic memory. Psychological Review, 80(5), 352.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1973). Availability: A heuristic for judging frequency and probability. Cognitive Psychology, 5(2), 207–232.
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1983). Extensional versus intuitive reasoning: The conjunction fallacy in probability judgment. Psychological Review, 90(4), 293.
United states v. greene (Vol. 704, p. 298). (2013). (Vol. 704). United States Court of Appeals, Fourth Circuit.
Von Frisch, K. (1950). Bees: Their vision, chemical senses, and language. Cornell University Press.
Von Restorff, H. (1933). Über die wirkung von bereichsbildungen im spurenfeld. Psychologische Forschung, 18(1), 299–342.
Wade, K. A., Garry, M., Don Read, J., & Lindsay, D. S. (2002). A picture is worth a thousand lies: Using false photographs to create false childhood memories. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 9(3), 597–603.
Wason, P. C. (1960). On the failure to eliminate hypotheses in a conceptual task. Q. J. Exp. Psychol., 12(3), 129–140.
Watkins, O. C., & Watkins, M. J. (1975). Buildup of proactive inhibition as a cue-overload effect. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1(4), 442.
Watson, J. M., & Strayer, D. L. (2010). Supertaskers: Profiles in extraordinary multitasking ability. Psychon. Bull. Rev., 17(4), 479–485.
Waxman, S. R. (1990). Linguistic biases and the establishment of conceptual hierarchies: Evidence from preschool children. Cogn. Dev., 5(2), 123–150.
Wellman, H. M. (1990). The child’s theory of mind. MIT Press.
Wellman, H. M., Cross, D., & Watson, J. (2001). Meta-analysis of theory-of-mind development: The truth about false belief. Child Development, 72, 655–684.
Wells, G. L., Kovera, M. B., Douglass, A. B., Brewer, N., Meissner, C. A., & Wixted, J. T. (2020). Policy and procedure recommendations for the collection and preservation of eyewitness identification evidence. Law and Human Behavior, 44(1), 3–36. https://doi.org/10.1037/lhb0000359
Wells, G. L., Memon, A., & Penrod, S. D. (2006). Eyewitness evidence: Improving its probative value. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 7(2), 45–75.
Wells, G. L., & Olson, E. A. (2003). Eyewitness testimony. Annual Review of Psychology, 54(1), 277–295.
Wells, G. L., Small, M., Penrod, S., Malpass, R. S., Fulero, S. M., & Brimacombe, C. A. E. (1998). Eyewitness identification procedures: Recommendations for lineups and photospreads. Law and Human Behavior, 22(6), 603–647. https://doi.org/10.1023/A:1025750605807
Werker, J. F., & Tees, R. C. (2002). Cross-language speech perception: Evidence for perceptual reorganization during the first year of life. Infant Behav. Dev., 25(1), 121–133.
Wertheimer, M. (1938). Gestalt theory. In W. D. Ellis (Ed.), A source book of gestalt psychology (pp. 1–11). Harcourt.
Wertheimer, M. (1945). Productive thinking. Harper New York.
Whitmore, N. W., Bassard, A. M., & Paller, K. A. (2022). Targeted memory reactivation of face-name learning depends on ample and undisturbed slow-wave sleep. NPJ Sci. Learn., 7(1), 1.
Wiley, J. (1998). Expertise as mental set: The effects of domain knowledge in creative problem solving. Memory & Cognition, 26, 716–730.
Williams, J. R., Markov, Y. A., Tiurina, N. A., & Störmer, V. S. (2022). What you see is what you hear: Sounds alter the contents of visual perception. Psychological Science, 09567976221121348.
Wilson, D. K., Kaplan, R. M., & Schneiderman, L. J. (1987). Framing of decisions and selections of alternatives in health care. Social Behaviour, 2(1), 51–59.
Wimmer, H., & Perner, J. (1983). Beliefs about beliefs: Representation and constraining function of wrong beliefs in young children’s understanding of deception. Cognition, 13, 103–128.
Winawer, J., Witthoft, N., Frank, M. C., Wu, L., Wade, A. R., & Boroditsky, L. (2007). Russian blues reveal effects of language on color discrimination. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 104(19), 7780–7785.
Winograd, E., Peluso, J. P., & Glover, T. A. (1998). Individual differences in susceptibility to memory illusions. Appl. Cogn. Psychol., 12(7), S5–S27.
Wixted, J. T. (2024). Atkinson and shiffrin’s (1968) influential model overshadowed their contemporary theory of human memory. Journal of Memory and Language, 136, 104471.
Wixted, J. T., Mickes, L., & Fisher, R. P. (2018). Rethinking the reliability of eyewitness memory. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 13(3), 324–335.
Wixted, J. T., & Wells, G. L. (2017). The relationship between eyewitness confidence and identification accuracy: A new synthesis. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 18(1), 10–65.
Wixted, J. T., Wells, G. L., Loftus, E. F., & Garrett, B. L. (2021). Test a witness’s memory of a suspect only once. Psychological Science in the Public Interest, 22(1_suppl), 1S–18S. https://doi.org/10.1177/15291006211026259
Woodward, A. L. (1998). Infants selectively encode the goal object of an actor’s reach. Cognition, 69, 1–34.
Yarbus, A. L. (1967). Eye movements and vision. Plenum Press.
Yilmaz, A. S., Shen, K. J., & Wixted, J. T. (2023). The principles of memory versus the federal rules of evidence.
Zhang, J., Hedden, T., & Chia, A. (2012). Perspective-taking and depth of theory-of-mind reasoning in sequential-move games. Cognitive Science, 36, 560–573.